Fill This Form To Receive Instant Help

Help in Homework
trustpilot ratings
google ratings


Homework answers / question archive / PSYC 565 ARTICLE REVIEW ASSIGNMENT INSTRUCTIONS OVERVIEW: The purpose of the Article Review Assignment is to guide you through some of the thought processes necessary to be a savvy consumer of primary sources of peer reviewed studies

PSYC 565 ARTICLE REVIEW ASSIGNMENT INSTRUCTIONS OVERVIEW: The purpose of the Article Review Assignment is to guide you through some of the thought processes necessary to be a savvy consumer of primary sources of peer reviewed studies

Psychology

PSYC 565 ARTICLE REVIEW ASSIGNMENT INSTRUCTIONS OVERVIEW: The purpose of the Article Review Assignment is to guide you through some of the thought processes necessary to be a savvy consumer of primary sources of peer reviewed studies. This exercise will help you determine not only the findings in the article but also the larger theoretical framework in which the author has placed those findings, the limitations of the work, and the impact it has had on the scientific community. By answering the following questions, you will also have a clear understanding of the development and rationale of the research hypothesis. Finally, this process will help you learn to communicate your own ideas more effectively in your Research Paper: Final Assignment and perhaps your thesis. INSTRUCTIONS: Submit the completed form and a PDF of the article being reviewed. See the example below for a detailed explanation of the required material for each question. 1. APA reference of the article being reviewed. Write the reference for the article as if it were in the reference section of your paper. 2. What is the research problem that is being investigated? What is the purpose of the research being conducted? Provide the “why” behind the paper. Why have they conducted this experiment? For example: “These experiments were designed to explore the role of second order conditioning in anxiety disorders.” 3. What is the research question? The research question is more specific. What is the specific question or questions the article will answer as a result of the study or experiment. For example: “Are adolescents more sensitive to the memory imparting effects of alcohol?” 4. How many sources are used in the introduction? List each one. Tell how many sentences are written about each source. Count the number of sources in the introduction and then list them. For each source give the number of sentences written about it. 5. Outline the introduction (Divide the introduction into thematic groups with their own subheadings—and include what information is provided about each source. The end of your outline should point you to the research question and design—these need to be included in your outline). Think about the introduction as a whole and then break it down into its thematic groups. Be sure to include the research question in the outline. I. Prevalence and cost of drug addiction Page 1 of 4 PSYC 565 II. III. IV. V. a. Global b. US Relapse a. Prevalence b. Description c. Cause d. Prevention Spontaneous Recovery a. Animal studies i. Dr. X first described it in rats ii. Theoretical explanation 1. Initial explanation is…. 2. Recent studies suggest that it is best explained in terms of ….. b. Human studies i. Consistent with animal findings. Spontaneous Recovery used to explain drug relapse a. Animal studies i. Stress ii. Small dose induction b. Human studies i. Stress ii. Small dose induction 1. Prescription pain medication Research question: Could stress management help reduce relapse in individuals recovering from substance us? a. Hypothesis of the writers b. Support for hypothesis c. Predicted results 6. What are two (or more) theories that are discussed in the introduction? How are they used to motivate (or set up) the research question? Do the authors agree or disagree with these theories? Simply restate the theories discussed in the introduction in your own words. State how these theories are driving the research questions. If the authors’ hypothesis is correct, will it support the theory or be inconsistent with the theory? You should have a good idea of where the authors stand based on the evidence presented and the arguments they are making. 7. What previous research (not theories) have been conducted on this topic? How do these previous studies relate to the current research question? Focus on the experiments cited in the introduction. In your own words, give a short summary of what has already been done and then explain how those findings relate to the current research question. Page 2 of 4 PSYC 565 8. Why is the proposed research the logical next step (that is—what rationale and motivation) is there for this research? What hole in psychological knowledge is this study following? Restate/summarize the authors’ rationale for the research in your own words. How is the current research question a logical progression of the previous findings? Answering this question will require that you think about the authors’ rationale for their research. They should have made a strong case for why this is the logical next step as well as pointed out that answering this question will fill in an important gap in the literature. 9. How is the research question operationalized? (First, identify the abstract constructs being studied. Next identify the concrete way these are being observed or measured. This should include your IV and DV.) A construct is an abstract explanatory variable that is not directly observable (e.g., memory). The concrete way the construct is measured will point you to the dependent variable (DV). For example, if the paper is concerned with memory, the DV may be the number of items recalled. The independent variable (IV) could be the amount of sleep each participant was allowed the night before the test. Remember that we cannot directly measure many of the constructs that are studied in psychology, so it is important that we identify how they are being operationalized in each research study. 10. What is the research design? (I.e., between or within subjects, what type of statistical tests were used, what were the levels of each variable?) This information will be in the methods section of your paper. Be sure to provide enough detail to describe how the study was designed. 11. Describe the results (but not their broader implications). Were the results significant? Which ones? Do these support or not support the hypothesis? Describe the results in your own words. For example: Group X was able to recall significantly more words than Group Y. This finding supports the hypothesis that manipulation Y would reduce recall. 12. Outline the discussion. Divide the discussion into thematic groups with their own subheadings. This should probably begin with a summary of the results and the previous sources. How many sources are used in the general discussion? Summarize the sources used and how they relate to the discussion and the research question. 13. What limitations are mentioned? Why are these limitations theoretically interesting? Limitations can be found in the discussion section of the paper. If a limitation is that they didn’t have X control group, then explain in your own words why that is important. Does does it change the interpretation of the findings? Page 3 of 4 PSYC 565 14. What future research is anticipated? Why is the future research theoretically interesting? Describe in your own words the future research that the author anticipates. Discuss why these studies would be of theoretical importance. How would they further our understanding of the construct or theory in question? 15. What is the Eigenfactor of the journal that published this article? Is it high or low? Do you think that the impact factor influences the quality of the article (or vise-versa)? The Eigenfactor is one measure of the impact of a journal. If you are unfamiliar with Eigenfactors, please see the Article Review Resources section for a short description. You can find the Eigenfactor for your article at the Eigenfactor website. Type in the name of your journal, find it in the list of results and report the EF value. Remember the total of all Eigenfactors is 100 and the highest individual Eigenfactor currently belongs to Science (1.22). 16. How many times has this article been cited? There are many ways to find this information. Some database site will give you this information or you can go to Web of Science and select a “Cited Reference Search”. This information should give you a rough idea of the impact this article has had on the field. See the Article Review Resources section under the Article Review Assignment page. Be sure to review the Article Review Grading Rubric before beginning the Article Review Assignment. Page 4 of 4 Child Development, July/August 2018, Volume 89, Number 4, Pages 1120–1132 Parent and Peer Predictors of Change in Attachment Security From Adolescence to Adulthood Joseph P. Allen, Leah Grande, Joseph Tan, and Emily Loeb University of Virginia Interview, self-report, peer report, and observational data were used to examine parent and peer relationship qualities as predictors of relative changes in attachment security in a community sample of adolescents followed from ages 14 to 24. Early maternal supportive behavior predicted relative increases in attachment security from adolescence to adulthood, whereas psychological control and interparental hostile con?ict predicted relative decreases. Peer predictors of relative increases in security included collaborative and autonomous behaviors and lack of hostile interactions, with peer predictions growing stronger for relationships assessed at later ages. Overall, models accounted for suf?cient variance as to suggest that attachment security across this period is well explained by a combination of stability plus theoretically predicted change linked to social relationship qualities. As the attachment behavioral system has increasingly been recognized as having lifelong functional relevance, the importance of understanding the development of this system into adulthood has become clear. A secure/autonomous adult attachment organization has been robustly linked not only to the security of one’s infant offspring but also to indices of adult mental health, romantic relationship quality, and broader social functioning (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985; Roisman, Madsen, Hennighausen, Sroufe, & Collins, 2001; Shlafer, Raby, Lawler, Hesemeyer, & Roisman, 2015; van IJzendoorn, 1995; van IJzendoorn & BakermansKranenburg, 1996). In considering the development of the attachment system from adolescence into adulthood, there is good reason to expect both stability as well as predictable change in levels of attachment security. By adolescence, internal models of relationships and patterns of relationship behavior have often become well established and may exist largely outside of conscious awareness (Bowlby, 1980; Fraley, 2002; Loeb, Hessel, & Allen, 2015). Yet, from a developmental perspective, adolescence is also a period of rapidly changing social relationships and rapidly increasing cognitive capacity, both of which This study and its write-up were supported by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the National Institute of Mental Health (9R01 HD058305-11A1 and R01-MH58066). Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joseph P. Allen, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904-4400. Electronic mail may be sent to allen@virginia.edu. may facilitate reworking of an existing attachment organization (Allen & Miga, 2010). Prior research suggests weak to moderate continuity in attachment security from infancy to late adolescence (Fraley, 2002; Groh et al., 2014), although greater stability has been found in short-term studies within adolescence (Allen, McElhaney, Kuperminc, & Jodl, 2004). Short-term change in adolescent attachment security has been linked to baseline levels of depression, lower family income, and enmeshed family con?ict (Allen et al., 2004). No research to date, however, has explored attachment stability or predictors of change from adolescence into adulthood, even though it is in adulthood that attachment security becomes most strongly predictive of future parenting behavior (Shlafer et al., 2015). The adolescent to adulthood transition thus becomes a critical link in building a life span and intergenerational understanding of the development of the attachment system. Cross-sectional correlations provide most of our current knowledge regarding the relation between attachment and social experiences in adolescence (see Dykas, Ziv, & Cassidy, 2008; Furman & Shomaker, 2008; Kobak, Cole, Ferenz-Gillies, Fleming, & Gamble, 1993; Reimer, Overton, Steidl, Rosenstein, & Horowitz, 1996). These correlations yield valuable information about the past interplay of attachment and relationship experiences, by © 2017 The Authors Child Development © 2017 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2018/8904-0006 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12840 Predictors of Attachment Security suggesting potential in?uences of relationship experiences on attachment security and vice versa. These correlations offer little insight, however, regarding factors that potentially in?uence, or at least predict, the development of attachment security going forward. To identify such factors we need to identify predictors of future relative changes in attachment from adolescence to adulthood—predictors that tell us something about attachment in the future, over and above what can be predicted from baseline attachment security. This study examined both cross-sectional correlations and predictors of relative change with regard to the two major sources of adolescent social experience: family relationships and peer relationships. In terms of family relationships, direct parental emotional support in childhood has long been linked to attachment security, and it is reasonable to expect such links to extend into adolescence (Allen et al., 2003; De Wolff & van Ijzendoorn, 1997). Other family experiences likely serve as indicators both of parents’ capacity for support and of their ability to model for the adolescent the possibility of establishing secure relationships beyond the family. For example, as parents handle con?ict with one another, they may model a goal-corrected process in which they continuously adapt their behavior to meet mutual goals—a process fundamental to maintenance of secure attachment relationships (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Kobak & Duemmler, 1994). Excessively harsh interparental con?ict, in contrast, presents teens with a model in which the goal-corrected process fails. Such con?ict may not only reduce parental availability to provide support for teens, it is also likely to undermine teens’ con?dence in their ability to establish their own secure, goal-corrected partnerships going forward (Steinberg, Davila, & Fincham, 2006). High levels of harsh interparental con?ict have been repeatedly linked to emotional insecurity in childhood and early adolescence (Davies & Cummings, 1998; Davies, Harold, Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002; Woodward, Fergusson, & Belsky, 2000). An additional family relationship factor—parents’ handling of adolescent autonomy strivings—also appears likely to be linked to security given the centrality of adolescent autonomy development as a precursor to development of healthy peer and romantic relationships into adulthood (Oudekerk, Allen, Hessel, & Molloy, 2015). Parental undermining of adolescent autonomy, for example, via psychologically controlling behavior, has been linked to short-term relative decreases in security within adolescence (Allen et al., 2004). Whether any of these family 1121 experiences actually predict the long-term development of attachment security into adulthood has not, however, been previously assessed. In terms of peer relationship predictors of the developing attachment system from adolescence into adulthood, perhaps the critical question is: At what age do peer relationships become suf?ciently intense and attachment focused that they begin not simply to re?ect, but also to potentially in?uence, or at least predict, the future development of the attachment system? Several lines of research suggest that early in adolescence, such intense and deep peer relationships are unlikely to exist (Furman, 2001; Rosenthal & Kobak, 2010; Zeifman & Hazan, 2008). In early adolescence, attachment organization may predict the quality of peer relationships, as it has been found to do at earlier ages (Pallini, Baiocco, Schneider, Madigan, & Atkinson, 2014), but it does not appear nearly so likely that the development of the attachment system will be predicted by such early relationships. As development progresses toward adulthood, however, both peer and romantic partner relationships increasingly take on qualities of true attachment relationships (Zeifman & Hazan, 2008). As these relationships become more intense, however, the challenge of managing them in ways that leave the adolescent feeling “autonomous, yet valuing of relationships”—a hallmark of adult security—becomes substantial (Allen & Manning, 2007; Allen, Porter, McFarland, McElhaney, & Marsh, 2007). Meeting this challenge requires forming relationships characterized by support and collaboration while avoiding hostile, relationship-undermining interactions when disagreements inevitably occur. Establishing adaptive peer and romantic partner autonomy and support processes is thus likely to be critical to the formation of secure relationships going forward and thus also to adult attachment security. Although adolescent peer relationships have been related to prior and concurrent attachment organization (Allen et al., 2004; Van Ryzin & Leve, 2012), they have never been assessed for their potential to explain the development of attachment security. In the current study, peer autonomy and support processes were thus assessed at three stages: in early adolescence (via assessments of popularity and friendship quality), in midadolescence (via assessments of autonomy and support processes with close friends), and in late adolescence (via assessments of autonomy and support processes with romantic partners). A ?nal important issue in assessing stability and change in adult attachment is the question of 1122 Allen, Grande, Tan, and Loeb whether variations in attachment security are best conceptualized and assessed as continuous versus discrete (i.e., categorical) entities (Fraley & Roisman, 2014). Given the psychometric advantages of a continuum approach (Fraley & Roisman, 2014; Haydon & Roisman, 2011, 2012), the current investigation primarily utilized continuum-based Q-sort methods to assess attachment. In adulthood, however, we also obtained categorical assessments of attachment security, which permits comparison of categorical and continuous approaches in accounting for continuities from previously assessed attachment and social experiences. To assess predictors of developing attachment security, this study examined parental and peer relationship correlates and predictors of relative change in attachment security from age 14 to age 24 in a diverse community sample to address the following questions: 1. How stable is attachment security from adolescence into adulthood? 2. How are support and autonomy processes in parent–adolescent interactions related to the development of attachment security from adolescence into adulthood? 3. How are support and autonomy in interactions with peers (both close friends and romantic partners) linked to current and future attachment, and do predictions change as development progresses? 4. To what extent are parent and peer relationship factors overlapping versus unique in their capacity to predict relative changes in attachment security across this period? 5. Is a categorical approach to assessing adult attachment security more versus less effective than a continuum approach in explaining continuities from prior attachment security and social relationship experiences? Method Participants This report is drawn from a larger longitudinal investigation of adolescent social development in familial and peer contexts. Analyses included 141 adolescents (61 male and 80 female) who received the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) at age 14 (M = 14.8, SD = 0.61) and again at age 24 (M = 24.7, SD = 0.92). Data were collected between January 2000 and June 2011. This ?nal sample comprised a subset of 174 teens who received the AAI at age 14 (81% reinterview rate). Adolescents’ interactions with parents were also observed at ages 13 and 16 (Ms = 13.3 and 16.3, SDs = 0.63 and 0.85, respectively). Interactions with a person named by the adolescent as the peer to whom they were closest were observed at ages 14 and 16/17 (Ms = 14.8, 16.3, and 17.3, SDs = 0.61, 0.85, and 0.85, respectively). Interactions with a romantic partner, for participants in relationships of at least 3 months duration, were assessed at age 21 (M = 20.9, SD = 1.08). The ?nal sample was racially/ethnically and socioeconomically diverse: 79 (56%) adolescents identi?ed themselves as Caucasian, 44 (31%) as African American, 2 (1%) as Asian, 3 (2%) as Hispanic, 1 (1%) as American Indian, and 12 (9%) as from other or mixed racial/ethnic groups. Adolescents’ parents reported a median family income at baseline in the $40,000–$59,999 range (M = $43,900, SD = $22,500). More detail about participant selection, data collection procedures, and attrition analyses are presented in Appendix S1. Measures Note that additional detail about measure content and psychometric characteristics of measures is presented in Appendix S1. AAI and Q-Set (Ages 14 and 24) This structured interview probes individuals’ descriptions of their childhood relationships with parents in both abstract terms and with requests for speci?c supporting memories (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996; Kobak et al., 1993). Slight adaptations to the adult version were made to make the questions more natural and easily understood for an adolescent population at age 14 (Ward & Carlson, 1995). Hence, comparisons in absolute ratings from the age 14 and age 24 interviews should be made only with great caution. The AAI Q-set (Kobak et al., 1993) was designed to closely parallel the AAI Classi?cation System (Main, Goldwyn, & Hesse, 2002) but to yield continuous measures of qualities of attachment organization. For the age 24 attachment assessments, coders also provided reliable formal classi?cations using the Main et al. (2002) system. The point–biserial correlation between the secure classi?cation and the continuous measure of security from the Q-sort was .84 (p < .001), indicating a high degree of correspondence between results from the two systems. Predictors of Attachment Security Maternal Supportive Behavior (Ages 13 and 16) Adolescents participated in an 8-min supportive behavior interaction task with their mothers, during which they were instructed to ask for help with “a problem they were having that they could use some advice or support about,” with interactions coded using the Supportive Behavior Coding System (Allen et al., 2001). Parental Psychological Control (Ages 14 and 16/17) Adolescents completed the Psychological Control versus Psychological Autonomy subscale of the Childhood Report of Parenting Behavior Inventory assessing the degree to which mothers and fathers use guilt, love withdrawal, or other autonomyundermining methods to control adolescents’ behavior (Schaefer, 1965; Schludermann & Schludermann, 1988). Scores from ages 16 and 17 assessments were averaged together to create ratings for the age 16/17 period. Interparental Hostile Con?ict (Age 13) Hostile con?ict between parents was reported by both parents and averaged together using the sixitem symbolic/psychological aggression scale from the Con?ict Tactics Scale (CTS; Straus, 1979). 1123 Close Peer Collaborative Behavior and Hostile Behavior (Ages 14, 16/17) Each adolescent-close peer dyad participated in an 8-min videotaped task in which they were presented with a hypothetical dilemma that involved making a series of discrete decisions about a social situation. The Autonomy-Relatedness Coding System for Peer Interactions was used to reliably code collaborative and hostile behavior in these interactions (Allen, Porter, & McFarland, 2001). Scores from ages 16 and 17 assessments were averaged together. Observed Romantic Partner Supportive Behavior (Age 21) Late adolescents participated in an 8-min interaction task with their romantic partner, during which they asked their partner for help with “a problem they were having that they could use some advice or support about,” with interactions coded using the Supportive Behavior Coding System (Allen et al., 2001). Romantic Partner Autonomy Support (Age 21) Autonomy support when disagreeing was assessed via romantic partner reports on the CTS (Straus, 1979). Popularity (Age 14) Adolescent popularity was assessed using a limited nomination sociometric procedure. Each adolescent, their closest friend, and two other target peers named by the adolescent were asked to nominate up to 10 peers in their grade with whom they would “most like to spend time on a Saturday night” and an additional 10 peers in their grade with whom they would “least like to spend time on a Saturday night.” Popularity was assessed as the number of nominations received by the participating adolescent (standardized within grade level). Close Peer Supportive Behavior (Ages 14, 16/17) Adolescents participated in an 8-min interaction task with their closest friend, during which they asked that peer for help with “a problem they were having that they could use some advice or support about,” with interactions coded using the Supportive Behavior Coding System (Allen et al., 2001). Scores from ages 16 and 17 assessments were averaged together. Results Preliminary Analyses Means and standard deviations for all substantive variables are presented in Table 1. For descriptive purposes, Table 2 presents simple univariate correlations (or point–biserial correlations for dichotomous variables) among the key variables of interest. Adolescent gender and family income were related to several variables in the study and hence were included as covariates in all analyses below. Potential moderating effects of gender or income were examined to assess whether results differed for male versus female adolescents or based on initial family income for the analyses below. No moderating effects were found. Primary Analyses Question 1: How stable is attachment security from adolescence into adulthood? 1124 Allen, Grande, Tan, and Loeb Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations AAI attachment security (age 14) AAI attachment security (age 24) Interparental hostile con?ict (age 13) Parental psychological control (age 14) Parental psychological control (age 16/17) Observed maternal support (age 13) Observed maternal support (age 16) Sociometric popularity (age 14) Observed collaborative peer behavior (age 14) Observed collaborative peer behavior (age 16/17) Observed peer hostility (age 14) Observed peer hostility (age 16/17) Observed peer support (age 14) Observed peer support (age 16/17) Observed romantic partner supportive (age 21) Romantic partner autonomy support (age 21) M SD 0.25 0.03 5.46 14.60 14.83 2.95 3.06 0.93 2.59 2.65 1.00 1.15 2.52 2.88 2.76 3.46 0.42 0.46 2.93 3.37 3.43 0.79 0.63 1.33 0.66 0.48 1.41 1.53 0.66 0.61 0.80 4.4 The simple correlation between attachment security at age 14 and age 24 was r = .46, p < .001. Security scores declined signi?cantly over this 10year period, Tmean change(1,133) = 5.24, p < .001, as seen in Table 1, although slight differences in attachment interview questions from adolescence to adulthood make scores across ages less than fully comparable. Question 2: How are support and autonomy processes in parent–adolescent interactions related to the development of attachment security from adolescence into adulthood? Analyses were designed to assess the relation of parent and peer relationship qualities to baseline attachment security and to predictions of attachment security at 24 after accounting for security at 14. This latter approach of predicting future security while accounting for predictions from baseline levels (e.g., stability) yields one marker of change: relative increases or decreases in security relative to predictions from baseline levels (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). We began by assessing whether the block of parent–adolescent relationship measures signi?cantly added to the explained variance in adult security after accounting for adolescent security and demographic factors. This block was highly signi?cant, v2(6) = 17.74, p = .007, and the overall model accounted for 36.2% of the variance in attachment security at 24 (p < .001), with family predictors accounting for an additional 11% of the variance, over and above demographic factors and baseline attachment security (see Table S1). We then assessed potential mediated pathways among observed family predictors of security using a path analytic approach in which all temporally possible paths among predictors were considered initially, with nonsigni?cant paths then deleted. Signi?cant paths in the ?nal model, which ?t the data well (goodness-of-?t index [GFI] = .99, adjusted GFI [AGFI] = .97, root mean square error of approximation [RMSEA] = 0), v2(5) = 1.93, p = .86, are depicted in Figure 1. Early adolescent maternal support and interparental hostile con?ict were both directly predictive of lower levels of security at age 24, after accounting for security at age 14. Parental psychological control at age 16/17 displayed continuity with psychological control in early adolescence and was also predictive of future security. Family demographic factors (e.g., income and presence of both biological parents in the home) were related to attachment security at age 14 but were not predictive of relative changes in security into adulthood. Question 3: How are support and autonomy in interactions with peers (both close friends and romantic partners) linked to current and future attachment, and do predictions change as development progresses? Using the same hierarchical regression approach described earlier, the block of peer and romantic partner relationship qualities signi?cantly added to the explained variance in adult attachment security after accounting for adolescent security and demographic factors, v2(9) = 32.41, p < .001. The overall model accounted for 44.3% of the variance in security at 24, with peer predictors accounting for an additional 19% of the variance over and above the contributions of demographic factors and baseline attachment security (see Table S2). Figure 2 depicts the signi?cant paths accounting for peer predictors from a path model created as described earlier. In this model, which ?t the data well (GFI = .98, AGFI = .91, RMSEA = 0), v2(25) = 16.11, p = .91, collaborative close peer behavior at age 16/17, romantic partner autonomy support at age 21, and lack of hostile close peer behavior in interaction tasks at age 16/17 each uniquely contributed to explaining future attachment security after considering baseline security and demographic factors. 2. 3. 12 24** 11 — 4. 35*** 16* 46*** 02 — 5. 06 20* 16 03 22* — 6. Note. Correlations are multiplied by 100. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 1. Attachment security (age 14) 46*** 28*** 2. Attachment security (age 24) — 13 3. Family income (age 13) — 4. Gender (M = 1, F = 2) 5. Living with biological parents 6. Interparental hostile con?ict (age 13) 7. Psychological control (age 14) 8. Psychological control (age 16/17) 9. Maternal support (age 13) 10. Maternal support (age 16) 11. Popularity (age 14) 12. Collaborative peer behavior (age 14) 13. Collaborative peer behavior (age 16/17) 14. Peer hostility (age 14) 15. Peer hostility (age 16/17) 16. Peer support (age 14) 17. Peer support (age 16/17) 18. Romantic partner support (age 21) 19. Romantic partner autonomy support (age 21) Table 2 Intercorrelations of Substantive Variables 35*** 29*** 17* 04 07 04 — 7. 29*** 33*** 07 03 06 21* 62*** — 8. 32*** 27** 38*** 06 31*** 01 12 07 — 9. 23* 21* 37*** 02 26** 19* 36*** 28*** 37*** — 10. 34*** 12 34*** 14 23*** 17 19* 20*** 12 23*** — 11. 14 06 01 18* 07 05 01 12 07 02 14 — 12. 04 33*** — 04 — — 13 24** 10 03 13 08 16* 30*** 07 02 15 14 15. 20* 17* 01 01 10 09 00 19* 14 08 22** 23** 14. 06 25** 18* 02 10 16 08 18* 10 12 15 30*** 13. 04 05 — 11 28*** 23** 13 26** 15 03 26** 19* 25** 24** 11 17* 16. 11 12 21* — 30*** 02* 16 05 09 13 08 11 03* 16 10 22** 13 17. 10 09 04 11 — 22* 27** 26* 13 25* 17 37*** 25* 18 10 04 37*** 15 18. 05 16 06 17 23* — 40*** 09 32*** 25** 03 19 14 01 00 01 13 16 19 19. Predictors of Attachment Security 1125 1126 Allen, Grande, Tan, and Loeb Age 16-17 Age 13-14 Age 24 Family Income Absence of Arrows Between Constructs Indicates non-significant Paths; Age 24 Attachment R2 = .36, p < .001. .28*** Live w/ Biol. Parents Baseline Correlates that do not Predict Change .35*** .34*** Secure/Autonomous Attachment Secure/Autonomous Attachment .16* .32*** .37*** Maternal Supportive Behav. Maternal Supportive Behav. -.19* -.32*** .58*** Parental Psychol. Control Parental Psychol. Control Predictors of Relative Change .19** .16* -.18* Gender Interparental Hostile Conflict Figure 1. Parent relationship predictors of attachment security. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. [Color ?gure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] Age 21 Age 16-17 Age 13-14 Age 24 .21* Family Income Gender .12* .28*** Hostile Close Peer Behavior .29*** Collaborative Close Peer Behavior .26*** Hostile Close Peer Behavior -.16* .37*** Romantic Partner Autonomy Support Collaborative Close Peer Behavior Predictors of Relave Change .21** -..15* .22** -.20* .22** .40*** Secure/Autonomous Attachment Secure/Autonomous Attachment .34*** Absence of Arrows Between Constructs Indicates non-signi?cant Paths; Age 24 Aachment R2 = .44, p < .001. Popularity .28*** Close Peer Supportive Behav. .20** Close Peer Supportive Behav. Romantic Partner Supportive Behav. Figure 2. Peer and romantic partner relationship predictors of attachment security. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. [Color ?gure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] Predictors of Attachment Security Question 4: To what extent are parent and peer predictors redundant or unique in their prediction of future attachment security? To assess the extent to which observed family and peer predictors were unique versus redundant, ?nal models were examined that included previously identi?ed signi?cant predictors of age 24 attachment security. We ?rst assessed whether peer relationship qualities added incremental variance over and above parent relationship qualities, baseline attachment, and demographic factors in explaining adult attachment security. Results indicated that the block of peer variables added signi?cant variance explained in adult attachment security, v2(5) = 25.43, p < .001 (see Table S3). A path model constructed as described above to examine mediated pathways explaining adult security ?t the data well (GFI = .99, AGFI = .95, RMSEA = 0), v2(15) = 6.69, p = .97. Results, depicted in Figure 3, indicated that all of the previously identi?ed close peer and romantic partner predictors of future attachment security remained as predictors. In contrast, only interparental hostile con?ict at age 14 remained as a unique family predictor of age 24 security (parental psychological control was nonsigni?cant in both regression and path models). The overall model accounted for 46.8% of the variance in attachment security at 24 (multiple R = .68, p < .001), with family and peer predictors together accounting for an additional 21.3% of the variance (p < .001), over and above the contributions of demographic factors and baseline attachment security. Question 5: Is a categorical approach to assessing adult attachment security more versus less effective than a continuum approach in explaining continuities from prior attachment and social relationship experiences? We next examined a series of models to consider whether either measure of adult attachment (i.e., continuous or categorical) displayed greater continuities with our predictor variables than the other measure. To do so, we examined simple correlations with our adolescent-era predictors. These correlations, presented in Table S4, reveal many predictors for which the two measures of security had near identical correlations. We tested the difference between the correlations using the Z score test for correlations from dependent samples with a single variable in common and found several cases in 1127 which signi?cant differences between correlations of the two attachment measures with prior predictors were observed. These predictors included prior attachment security, collaborative close peer behavior at ages 16 and 17, observed support from a close peer at ages 16 and 17 and from a romantic partner at age 21, and romantic partner autonomy support at age 21. In no case did the categorical approach demonstrate a stronger correlation with predictors than the continuous measure of adult attachment security. Discussion This study identi?ed a range of adolescent-era social relationship factors linked to attachment across a 10-year period from early adolescence to early adulthood. Attachment organization across this period displayed a level of stability considerably greater than has been observed from infancy into adolescence (Hamilton, 2000; Sroufe, 2005; Wein?eld, Whaley, & Egeland, 2004), suggesting that the adolescent to early adult span is one in which attachment organization may become relatively consolidated and less easily altered. Even given this observed stability, however, relationship experience factors yielded sizable contributions to explaining relative changes in attachment organization. This suggests that the individual’s attachment organization also potentially remains open to environmental in?uences during this period. In terms of parent–adolescent relationship characteristics, we observed both cross-sectional correlates of attachment within adolescence as well as several relationship factors that predicted relative change in attachment over the following decade. Maternal support in early adolescence was both a cross-sectional correlate of security at age 14, as well as a predictor of further relative gains in security from adolescence to adulthood. This is consistent with a long line of research at earlier stages of development on the fundamental importance of maternal responsiveness and sensitivity to the child’s needs (De Wolff & van Ijzendoorn, 1997). These ?ndings suggest that the importance of maternal support continues well into adolescence and indeed remains a predictor of future security even after accounting for the growing role of peer and romantic relationship factors during this period. Parental psychologically controlling (vs. autonomy-promoting) behavior was also predictive of 1128 Allen, Grande, Tan, and Loeb Age 13-14 .28*** Age 16-17 Age 24 .20* Family Income Gender Collaborative Close Peer Behavior Collaborative Close Peer Behavior Hostile Close Peer Behavior Hostile Close Peer Behavior .36*** .32*** Romantic Partner Autonomy Support -.16* .19** .19** .32*** Secure/Autonomous Attachment Maternal Supportive Behav. Age 21 .24** Secure/Autonomous Attachment .16* .23** Maternal Supportive Behav. -.32*** Parental Psychol. Control .58*** Parental Psychol. Control Interparental Hostile Conflict -.21** Absence of Arrows Between Constructs Indicates non-signi?cant Paths; Age 24 Aachment R2 = .50, p < .001. Figure 3. Combined parent and peer model predicting attachment security. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. [Color ?gure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] relative decreases in attachment security over time. It is likely not coincidental that the adult attachment organization that is most closely linked to infant security has been labeled as “autonomous, valuing of attachment” (Hesse, 2008). Adolescence is a period during which establishing the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral autonomy necessary to achieve or maintain a secure attachment organization is likely critical (Keating, 1990). Parental behaviors that undermine midadolescent autonomy go directly against this developmental imperative. This ?nding is also consistent with prior research showing links of autonomy struggles during middle adolescence to short-term changes in attachment security (Allen et al., 2004). The observed role of interparental hostile con?ict in predicting relative change in attachment security has several possible explanations. Such hostile con?ict may directly undermine adolescents’ sense of felt security as it appears to do in childhood (Davies & Cummings, 1998; Davies et al., 2002). Interparental hostility may also present the adolescent with a problematic model for the resolution of goal con?icts between partners—a model that may leave the adolescent questioning whether he or she can establish secure goal-corrected relationships going forward (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Kobak & Duemmler, 1994). Consistent with this explanation, prior research has found that high levels of enmeshed con?ict between parent and adolescent predict future insecurity within adolescence (Allen et al., 2004). Observing parents model such con?ict with one another may play a similar role. Overall, maternal supportive behavior, maternal psychological control, and interparental hostility accounted for 11% of the variance in attachment in adulthood, even after considering baseline attachment and demographic factors, suggesting that family experiences play an important role in understanding the development of the attachment system across the adolescent to adult transition. In contrast, family demographic factors (e.g., the experience of living with both biological parents and total family income) were signi?cantly related to attachment security at age 14 but did not predict future change beyond that point. Predictors of Attachment Security In terms of peer relationship predictors of attachment security, several clear patterns emerged. Assessments of the overall quality of af?rmation that an individual received from peers (i.e., popularity) was concurrently linked to security at age 14 but displayed no predictive value beyond that. In contrast, several markers of capacity to establish autonomy in peer relationships later in adolescence were not only linked to baseline attachment but predicted relative changes in attachment security over time. Attachment security may facilitate the successful management of peer relationships in early adolescence, just as it does at earlier points in development (Pallini et al., 2014); it may only be as peer relationships deepen, however, that qualities of these relationships become direct predictors of future attachment security. Notably, the strongest predictions of adult security were observed from age 21 con?ict management as the domain of observed peer relationships shifted to romantic relationships. Overall, these ?ndings are consistent with a developmental perspective on the growing importance of peer relationships during this span as adolescents begin turning to peers to meet attachment needs (Collins, 1997; Collins, van Dulmen, Arnett, & Tanner, 2006; Rosenthal & Kobak, 2010). As with parent predictors, the importance of autonomy processes in managing con?ict in peer relationships became increasingly apparent. In adulthood, establishing a secure attachment organization may depend to a large extent on establishing close relationships in which autonomy is supported and disagreements between partners can be resolved successfully. Although powerful, biologically based caregiving and pair-bonding systems would tend to promote support between partners (Cassidy, 2000; Zeifman & Hazan, 2008), the ability to manage disagreements so as not to create anger and hostility that undermine these systems may be central to adult security. An important caveat to the peer relationship ?ndings observed is that predictions from mid- and late adolescence were obtained after the baseline attachment assessment at age 14. These data thus cannot rule out the possibility that these later predictions may in part re?ect the in?uence of evolving attachment organization after age 14 and prior to the age 16/17 and age 21 assessments. These predictions, however, were obtained not from the adolescent’s behavior but from observations or partner reports of a peer or romantic partner’s behavior, which at least reduces the likelihood that the predictions solely re?ect the adolescent’s concurrent, unmeasured attachment organization. 1129 When family and peer predictors were considered together, each domain added unique variance to predictions from the other domain. All prior peer predictors remained signi?cant. In terms of family predictors, maternal psychological control dropped out as a unique predictor of future insecurity, whereas interparental hostile con?ict and early maternal supportive behavior remained. Even after accounting for the stability of attachment across this period, predictions from relational experience factors accounted for an additional 21% of the variance. These factors, together with predictions from baseline attachment assessments at 14, explained 46.8% of the total variance, a multiple R of .68, in adult attachment security—a level of explained variance that approaches the theoretical maximum given the limits of coding reliability. Attachment security in adulthood thus appears to be quite lawfully explained via a combination of prior attachment and key social relationship experiences. Data from this study also made it possible to address the question of how a categorical approach to assessing adult attachment might fare relative to a continuum approach in accounting for continuities from the adolescent era. A continuum approach was found to demonstrate equivalent or slightly stronger degrees of continuity to adolescent-era predictors than a categorical approach in relation to a number of speci?c relationship qualities assessed in adolescence. In no cases did the categorical approach display stronger relations to prior predictors than the continuum approach. Findings regarding assessments of stability are of course somewhat limited because in adolescence, we only had a continuous measure of security for comparison. Overall, however, this pattern of ?ndings provides signi?cant, though by no means dispositive, evidence supporting the greater utility of a continuous variable approach to assessing adult security. Several additional limitations to the approach used in this study also warrant consideration. One important quali?cation is that even lagged longitudinal data are not suf?cient to establish the presence of causal relationships between predictors and attachment outcomes. Similarly, mediated paths in path analytic models were not speci?ed a priori and hence should be considered exploratory in nature. In addition, when comparing stability observed in this study to stability observed in attachment security across other periods of the life span, it is important to note important conceptual and measurement shifts that occur and make comparisons of stability rates across different ages 1130 Allen, Grande, Tan, and Loeb problematic. Adult attachment organization conceptually differs from infant security in that it re?ects a generalized cognitive representation of attachment as opposed to a model of a speci?c relationship (Allen & Manning, 2007; Allen & Tan, 2016). The long-term relative stability of attachment in this study likely in part re?ected the use of the same measurement strategy over time, which is often impossible when stability is assessed across other developmental periods (e.g., from infancy to adolescence). Our measurement approach was also limited to approaches (e.g., of family interaction qualities) available at the time of baseline assessments. Also, given that most participants were not living with parents at age 21, it was not feasible, nor necessarily sensible to obtain observed interaction data with parents at this age. This does, however, of necessity preclude conclusions about parent–adult child predictors of adult attachment. Similarly, we were not able to obtain suf?cient participation from fathers in observational tasks to permit assessment of their roles and this would clearly be valuable for future studies to include. Finally, this study focused primarily on security versus insecurity, given the large body of research linking security to social functioning. However, approaches that considered subtypes of insecurity (e.g., unresolved, preoccupied, and dismissing classi?cations) could be pro?tably explored in future research. References Allen, J. P., Hall, F. D., Insabella, G. M., Land, D. J., Marsh, P. A., & Porter, M. R. (2001). Supportive behavior coding system. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia. Allen, J. P., & Manning, N. (2007). From safety to affect regulation: Attachment from the vantage point of adolescence. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, 117, 23–39. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.192 Allen, J. P., McElhaney, K. B., Kuperminc, G. P., & Jodl, K. M. (2004). Stability and change in attachment security across adolescence. Child Development, 75, 1792– 1805. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00817.x Allen, J. P., McElhaney, K. B., Land, D. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Moore, C. M., O’Beirne-Kelley, H., & Liebman, S. M. (2003). A secure base in adolescence: Markers of attachment security in the mother-adolescent relationship. Child Development, 74, 292–307. https://doi.org/10. 1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00536 Allen, J. P., & Miga, E. M. (2010). Attachment in adolescence: A move to the level of emotion regulation. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27, 181–190. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407509360898 Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., & McFarland, C. F. (2001). The autonomy and relatedness coding system for peer interactions. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia. Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., McFarland, F. C., McElhaney, K. B., & Marsh, P. A. (2007). The relation of attachment security to adolescents’ paternal and peer relationships, depression, and externalizing behavior. Child Development, 78, 1222–1239. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14678624.2007.01062.x Allen, J. P., & Tan, J. (2016). The multiple facets of attachment in adolescence. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (3rd ed., pp. 399–415). New York NY: Guilford. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness and depression. New York, NY: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Basic Books. (Original work published 1969) Cassidy, J. (2000). Adult romantic attachments: A developmental perspective on individual differences. Review of General Psychology, 4, 111–131. https://doi.org/10. 1037/1089-2680.4.2.111 Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Collins, W. A. (1997). Relationships and development during adolescence: Interpersonal adaptation to individual change. Personal Relationships, 4(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1997.tb00126.x Collins, A., van Dulmen, M., Arnett, J. J., & Tanner, J. L. (2006). Friendships and romance in emerging adulthood: Assessing distinctiveness in close relationships. In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 219–234). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1998). Exploring children’s emotional security as a mediator of the link between marital relations and child adjustment. Child Development, 69, 124–139. https://doi.org/10.2307/ 1132075 Davies, P. T., Harold, G. T., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Cummings, E. M. (2002). Child emotional security and interparental con?ict. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(3), vii–viii. De Wolff, M., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571– 591. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.x Dykas, M. J., Ziv, Y., & Cassidy, J. (2008). Attachment and peer relations in adolescence. Attachment & Human Development, 10, 123–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 14616730802113679 Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 123–151. https://doi.org/10.1207/ s15327957pspr0602_03 Predictors of Attachment Security Fraley, R. C., & Roisman, G. I. (2014). III. Categories or dimensions? A taxometric analysis of the Adult Attachment Interview. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(3), 36–50. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/0265407501185002 Furman, W. (2001). Working models of friendships. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships, 18, 583–602. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407501185002 Furman, W., & Shomaker, L. B. (2008). Patterns of interaction in adolescent romantic relationships: Distinct features and links to other close relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 31, 771–788. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ad olescence.2007.10.007 George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult Attachment Interview (3rd ed.). Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA. Groh, A. M., Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Fraley, R. C., Owen, M. T., Cox, M. J., & Burchinal, M. R. (2014). IV. Stability of attachment security from infancy to late adolescence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(3), 51–66. Hamilton, C. E. (2000). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 71, 690–694. https://doi.org/10.1111/ 1467-8624.00177 Haydon, K. C., Collins, W. A., Salvatore, J. E., Simpson, J. A., & Roisman, G. I. (2012). Shared and distinctive origins and correlates of adult attachment representations: the developmental organization of romantic functioning. Child Dev, 83, 1689–1702. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1467-8624.2012.01801.x. Haydon, K. C., Roisman, G. I., Marks, M. J., & Fraley, R. C. (2011). An empirically derived approach to the latent structure of the Adult Attachment Interview: additional convergent and discriminant validity evidence. Attach Hum Dev, 13, 503–524. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 14616734.2011.602253. Hesse, E. (2008). The Adult Attachment Interview: Protocol, method of analysis, and empirical studies. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 552–598). New York, NY: Guilford. Keating, D. P. (1990). Adolescent thinking. In G. R. Elliott S. S. Feldman (Eds.), At the threshold: the developing adolescent (pp. 54–89). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kobak, R. R., Cole, H., Ferenz-Gillies, R., Fleming, W., & Gamble, W. (1993). Attachment and emotion regulation during mother-teen problem-solving: A control theory analysis. Child Development, 64, 231–245. https://doi. org/10.2307/1131448 Kobak, R. R., & Duemmler, S. (1994). Attachment and conversation: Toward a discourse analysis of adolescent and adult security. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Attachment processes in adulthood. Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 5, pp. 121–149). London, UK: Jessica Kingsley. 1131 Loeb, E. L., Hessel, E. T., & Allen, J. P. (2015). The selfful?lling prophecy of adolescent social expectations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40, 555– 564. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415618274 Main, M., Goldwyn, R., & Hesse, E. (2002). Adult attachment scoring and classi?cation systems, Version 7.1. Unpublished Manuscript, University of California, Berkeley, CA. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(Serial No. 209), 66–104. Oudekerk, B. A., Allen, J. P., Hessel, E. T., & Molloy, L. E. (2015). The cascading development of autonomy and relatedness from adolescence to adulthood. Child Development, 86, 472–485. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev. 12313 Pallini, S., Baiocco, R., Schneider, B. H., Madigan, S., & Atkinson, L. (2014). Early child–parent attachment and peer relations: A meta-analysis of recent research. Journal of Family Psychology, 28, 118. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/a0035736 Reimer, M. S., Overton, W. F., Steidl, J. H., Rosenstein, D. S., & Horowitz, H. (1996). Familial responsiveness and behavioral control: In?uences on adolescent psychopathology, attachment, and cognition. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 87–112. Roisman, G. I., Madsen, S. D., Hennighausen, K. H., Sroufe, L. A., & Collins, W. A. (2001). The coherence of dyadic behavior across parent-child and romantic relationships as mediated by the internalized representation of experience. Attachment and Human Development, 3, 156–172. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730126483 Rosenthal, N. L., & Kobak, R. (2010). Assessing adolescents’ attachment hierarchies: Differences across developmental periods and associations with individual adaptation. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, 678– 706. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00655.x Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child Development, 36, 413–424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1965.tb05305.x Schludermann, E., & Schludermann, S. (1988). Children’s report on parent behavior (CRPBI-108, CRPBI-30) for older children and adolescents (Tech. Rep.). Winnipeg, MB: Department of Psychology, University of Manitoba. Shlafer, R. J., Raby, K. L., Lawler, J. M., Hesemeyer, P. S., & Roisman, G. I. (2015). Longitudinal associations between adult attachment states of mind and parenting quality. Attachment & Human Development, 17, 83–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.962064 Sroufe, L. A. (2005). Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attachment and Human Development, 7, 349–367. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500365928 Steinberg, S. J., Davila, J., & Fincham, F. (2006). Adolescent marital expectations and romantic experiences: 1132 Allen, Grande, Tan, and Loeb Associations with perceptions about parental con?ict and adolescent attachment security. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 333–348. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s10964-006-9042-9 Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily con?ict and violence: The Con?ict Tactics (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage & the Family, 41, 75–88. van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 387– 403. van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1996). Attachment representations in mothers, fathers, adolescents, and clinical groups: A meta-analytic search for normative data. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 8–21. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022006x.64.1.8 Van Ryzin, M. J., & Leve, L. D. (2012). Validity evidence for the Security Scale as a measure of perceived attachment security in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 425–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.07.014 Ward, M. J., & Carlson, E. A. (1995). Associations among adult attachment representations, maternal sensitivity, and infant-mother attachment in a sample of adolescent mothers. Child Development, 66, 69–79. Wein?eld, N. S., Whaley, G. J. L., & Egeland, B. (2004). Continuity, discontinuity, and coherence in attachment from infancy to late adolescence: Sequelae of organization and disorganization. Attachment and Human Development, 6, 73–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/146167303 10001659566 Woodward, L., Fergusson, D. M., & Belsky, J. (2000). Timing of parental separation and attachment to parents in adolescence: Results of a prospective study from birth to age 16. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 162– 174. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00162.x Zeifman, D., & Hazan, C. (2008). Pair bonds as attachments: Reevaluating the evidence. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 436–455). New York, NY: Guilford. Supporting Information Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher’s website: Appendix S1. Extended Description of Procedures, Measures, and Attrition Analyses Table S1. Family Predictors of Attachment Security (Age 24) After Accounting for Security at Age 14 Table S2. Peer Predictors of Attachment Security (Age 24) After Accounting for Security at Age 14 Table S3. Combined Peer and Family Predictors of Attachment Security (Age 24) After Accounting for Security at Age 14 Table S4. Comparing Continuous and Categorical Measures of Attachment Security in Terms of Their Relation to Adolescent-Era Predictors This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material.
 

pur-new-sol

Purchase A New Answer

Custom new solution created by our subject matter experts

GET A QUOTE

Answer Preview

Article Review

This is an article review based on the questions

 

  1. APA citation
  2. The purpose of the study
  3. Research question.
  4. Sources used in the introduction
  5. Thematic sections.
  6. Theories

9.  The operationalizing of research question.

10. Research design

11. Results

a. preliminary analyses results.

b. primary analysis.

12. Outline the discussion.

13. Study limitations.

14. Future research

15. The Eigenfactor of the journal

16. Times cited

  1.  

Allen, J. P., Grande, L., Tan, J., & Loeb, E. (2018). Parent and peer predictors of change in attachment security from adolescence to adulthood. Child Development89(4), 1120-1132.

  1. The purpose of the study

The experiments were designed to investigate parents and peer relationships qualities as they are used to foretell the relative change in attachment security of an individual as he grows up to adulthood. According to the research, early maternal aid escalates in attachment security as one transforms from adolescence to adulthood, on the contrary, psychological controls like hostility and conflict predict a subsided attachment security.

  1. Research question.

What is the parent and peer relationship behaviour that results in a relative change in attachment security of individuals?

  1. Sources used in the introduction

The research used five sources.                            

  1. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the society for research in child development, 66-104.

The research used one sentence from this source.

  1. Roisman, G. I., Madsen, S. D., Hennighausen, K. H., Alan oufe Sr, L., & Andrew Collins, W. (2001). The coherence of dyadic behaviour across parent-child and romantic relationships is mediated by the internalized representation of experience. Attachment & Human Development3(2), 156-172.

 The research used one sentence from this source.

 

  1. Shlafer, R. J., Raby, K. L., Lawler, J. M., Hesemeyer, P. S., & Roisman, G. I. (2015). Longitudinal associations between adult attachment states of mind and parenting quality. Attachment & human development17(1), 83-95.

The research used one sentence from this source.

  1. Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: a meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin117(3), 387.

The research used one sentence from this source.

 

  1. Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1996). Attachment representations in mothers, fathers, adolescents, and clinical groups: A meta-analytic search for normative data. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology64(1), 8.

The research used one sentence from this source.

  1. Thematic sections.

The first section address attachment behavioural system effect on functional relevance. The other section address how secure adult attachment modes linked with the security of one's young ones and adults mental health and general social functioning. The last section address how the transition to adulthood is prone to changes in the levels of attachment securities.

  1. Theories

The attachment behavioural system has escalated in having a function that is relevant and how the comprehension of the system development is known. secure adults attachment organization relates to the security of one infant and also the adult mental health, romantic interrelation, and general social functioning. In the last part of the theory address, one anticipates both stabilities and changes in attachments security levels.

9.  The operationalizing of the research question.

The dependent variable (DV) in the research is the change in attachment security of the individual; the ability of the person to make rational decisions and act per relevant attachment security. The independent variables (IV) for the research are the conditions that one is engaged in that shape the relevant attachment security behaviour. For example, psychological control and interparental hostile conflict.

10. Research design

Research method incorporated investigation done on adolescent social development in both peer context and familial. The research used 141 individuals who were interviewed. The study was also done on an ethnically different individual to account for diversity.  Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) was used in interviews with the participants which was done to a different set of ages of the participants. The AAI interview included interview done on the individual aged between 14 to 24 years, maternal supportive Behavoir aged between 13 and 16 years, parental psychological control on people aged between 14 to 17 years, interparental hostile conflict on people aged 13, popularity on people aged  14 years, close peer supportive behaviour aged 14 to 17, close peer collaborative behaviour and hostile behaviour on individual aged 14 to 17 years, romantic partner supportive behaviour on people aged 22, and romantic partner autonomy supports on people aged above 20

11. Results

a. preliminary analyses results.

The results indicated that potentials moderating impacts of gender income are seen whether the results were different for female versus male. No moderating was found.

b. primary analysis.

The research surveyed if peer correlation qualities subsidize increase variance over and above parents relationship qualities, baseline attachments, and demographics factor in explaining adult attachment securities. Outcomes indicated that the blocks of peer variable were significant variances explained in adults attachment. The results were significant for they support the hypothesis.

12. Outline the discussion.

The research sees varieties of adolescent eras of relationship factor connected to attachment dealing timeline of 10 years from adolescent to adulting. Attachments organization across the periods exhibited a greater level of stability as compared to that observed in infancy to adolescence. This observation implying that adolescent early adults are one in which attachments organizations may become consolidated and less altered? The observation suggested that the individual attachment organization remains open to environmental influences. The sources used were Hamilton  (2000), Sroufe  (2005), whale & Egeland  (2004) that suggested that adolescence is adolescence to early adulthood is a period that attachment organization becomes less altered. Another source is de wolf & van Ilzendoorn (1997) that indicated that the importance of maternal support continues well into the adolescent stage and romantic factors during the period. Another source was by Hesse (2008) that suggested that adult attachment organization is linked to infant security and known as autonomous valuing of attachment. Another source was by Allen which showed the consistent with previous findings. Another source was by Davies & Cummings 1998 and Davies (2002) which addresses the interparental hostile conflict that significantly affects the attachment organization of people. Another source was by Bowlby, 1969/1982; Kobak & Duemmler, 1994) that address the decisions of adolescents making secure goal-oriented relationship. Another source was presented by Pallini which states that attachment security makes successful management of peer relationship. Another source was by Cassidy, (2000) and Zeifman & Hazan, (2008), which insinuates the power to manage disputes that may arise to subside the extent of anger and state of being hostile undermine the systems that maybe be central adult security.

13. Study limitations.

The limitation that emerged from the study included a lagged longitudinal data that was insufficient to establish the presence of a relationship between the predictors and the outcomes. Another limitation was mediated path in the path analysis model which was unspecified in timing hence was, more exploratory. Another limitation that emerged was the comparison of the state and stability of the reach attachment secured in other times of the period of investigation. Researchers have to be keen on measurement and span between measurement to make comparisons of stability extent in distinct ages the determiners of attachment security. The limitation is significany6 for it contains the research from achieving its objectives.

14. Future research

During the research, some limitation was experienced. The measurement approach limitations like the qualities of family interaction in a period of the survey. Also since people aged 21 do not live with their parents it was hard to show their interaction with the parents. The study also addressed security versus insecurity as most research linked to social functioning. This led to many unresolved, dismissing, and preoccupied classification. Future research will attempt to fill the unsolved classification as mention above.

15. The Eigenfactor of the journal

The Eigenfactor published in the journal is high. This has a significant effect on the credibility of the article. The high ratings escalate the number of references to the same article thus bearing credible information.

16. Allen, J. P., Grande, L., Tan, J., & Loeb, E. (2018). Parent and peer predictors of change in attachment security from adolescence to adulthood. Child Development89(4), 1120-1132.

 Has been cited 23 times.

 

 

 

 

 

 

                                                                                                                                                           

Related Questions